Information

Literature with regards to facial attractiveness?

Literature with regards to facial attractiveness?

I would like to ask for suggestions of literature regarding the study of facial features, preferably in a context of how they may be considered attractive or not.

To clarify, the goal is to try and gain an understanding of what distinct facial features exist, and of what variation exists regarding each of them (in lengths, angles, other topographical aspects), and of how those features combine into more visually complex features, etc. This is for no particular purpose other than just my own curiosity.


Facial attractiveness: evolutionary based research

Face preferences affect a diverse range of critical social outcomes, from mate choices and decisions about platonic relationships to hiring decisions and decisions about social exchange. Firstly, we review the facial characteristics that influence attractiveness judgements of faces (e.g. symmetry, sexually dimorphic shape cues, averageness, skin colour/texture and cues to personality) and then review several important sources of individual differences in face preferences (e.g. hormone levels and fertility, own attractiveness and personality, visual experience, familiarity and imprinting, social learning). The research relating to these issues highlights flexible, sophisticated systems that support and promote adaptive responses to faces that appear to function to maximize the benefits of both our mate choices and more general decisions about other types of social partners.

References

. 1986 Preferences in human mate selection . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 50, 559–570.doi:

Langlois J. H., Kalakanis L., Rubenstein A. J., Larson A., Hallamm M.& Smoot M.

. 2000 Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review . Psychol. Bull. 126, 390–423.doi:

. 1994 Sexual selection . Princeton, NJ : Princeton University Press . Crossref, Google Scholar

. 1998 Bilateral symmetry and sexual selection: a meta-analysis . Am. Nat. 151, 174–192.doi:

. 1969 Appearance and education in marriage mobility . Am. Soc. Rev. 34, 519–533.doi:

. 1938 Personal appearance as related to scholastic records and marriage selection in college women . Hum. Biol. 10, 65–76. Google Scholar

. 1984 The role of non-verbal and physical attractiveness in the selection of dating partners . J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 1, 347–357.doi:

Berscheid E., Dion K., Walster E.& Walster G. W.

. 1971 Physical attractiveness and dating choice: a test of the matching hypothesis . J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 7, 173–189.doi:

Walster E., Aronson V., Abrahams D.& Rottman L.

. 1966 Importance of physical attractiveness in dating behaviour . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 4, 508–516.doi:

Dion K., Berscheid E.& Walster E.

. 1972 What is beautiful is good . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 24, 285–290.doi:

Eagly A. H., Ashmore R. D., Makhijani M. G.& Longo L. C.

. 1991 What is beautiful is good, but …: a meta-analytic review of research on the physical attractiveness stereotype . Psychol. Bull. 110, 109–128.doi:

. 1992 Good-looking people are not what we think . Psychol. Bull. 111, 304–341.doi:

. 1985 The aye of the beholder: susceptibility to sexism and beautyism in the evaluation of managerial applicants . J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 15, 591–605.doi:

. 1975 Beautiful but dangerous: effects of offender attractiveness and nature of the crime on juridical judgement . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 31, 410–414.doi:

. 1991 Natural observations of the links between attractiveness and initial legal judgments . Pers. Soc. Psychol. B 17, 541–547.doi:

. 2002 The relative importance of facial attractiveness and gender in Hong Kong selection decisions . Int. J. Hum. Resour. Manage. 13, 141–155.doi:

Marlowe C. M., Schneider S. L.& Nelson C. E.

. 1996 Gender and attractiveness biases in hiring decisions: are more experienced managers less biased? J. Appl. Psychol. 81, 11–21.doi:

. 1991 The beauty myth . New York, NY : Morrow . Google Scholar

. 1757 Four dissertations. IV: Of the standard of taste . London, UK : Millar . Google Scholar

. 1871 The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex . London, UK : John Murray . Google Scholar

. 1974 Physical attractiveness . Advances in experimental social psychology (ed.

), pp. 157–215. New York, NY : Academic Press . Google Scholar

. 1951 Patterns of sexual behaviour . New York, NY : Harper & Row . Google Scholar

Cunningham M. R., Roberts A. R., Barbee A. P.& Druen P. B.

. 1995 ‘Their ideas of beauty are, on the whole, the same as ours’: consistency and variability in the cross-cultural perception of female attractiveness . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 68, 261–279.doi:

Zebrowitz-McArthur L.& Baron R. M.

. 1983 Toward and ecological approach to social perception . Psychol. Rev. 90, 215–238.doi:

Thornhill R.& Gangestad S. W.

. 1999 Facial attractiveness . Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 452–460.doi:

. 2006 The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty . Ann. Rev. Psychol. 57, 199–226.doi:

. 1990 Relationships between blood parasites, mating success and phenotypic cues in male sage grouse . Am. Zool. 30, 271–278. Crossref, Google Scholar

. 1997 Developmental stability and fitness: a review . Am. Nat. 149, 916–932.doi:

. 1997 Asymmetry, developmental stability, and evolution . Oxford, UK : Oxford University Press . Google Scholar

. 1962 A study of fluctuating asymmetry . Evolution 16, 125–142.doi:

Dufour K. W.& Weatherhead P. J.

. 1998 Bilateral symmetry and social dominance in captive male red-winged blackbirds . Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 42, 71–76.doi:

Manning J. T., Scutt D.& Lewis-Jones D. I.

. 1998 Developmental stability, ejaculate size, and sperm quality in men . Evol. Hum. Behav. 19, 273–282.doi:

Manning J. T., Scutt D., Whitehouse G. H.& Leinster S. J.

. 1997 Breast asymmetry and phenotypic quality in women . Evol. Hum. Behav. 18, 223–236.doi:

Møller A. P., Soler M.& Thornhill R.

. 1995 Breast asymmetry, sexual selection, and human reproductive success . Ethol. Sociobiol. 16, 207–219. Crossref, Google Scholar

Thornhill R.& Gangestad S. W.

. 2006 Facial sexual dimorphism, developmental stability, and susceptibility to disease in men and women . Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 131–144.doi:

Gangestad S. W.& Thornhill R.

. 2003 Facial masculinity and fluctuating asymmetry . Evol. Hum. Behav. 24, 231–241.doi:

Little A. C., Jones B. C., Waitt C., Tiddeman B. P., Feinberg D. R., Perrett D. I., Apicella C. L., Marlowe F. W.& Reimchen T.

. 2008 Symmetry is related to sexual dimorphism in faces: data across culture and species . PLoS ONE 3, e2106.doi:

. 1997 Developmental stability and fitness: a review . Am. Nat. 149, 916–942.doi:

Thornhill R.& Gangestad S. W.

. 1994 Human fluctuating asymmetry and sexual behaviour . Psychol. Sci. 5, 297–302.doi:

. 1994 Human (Homo sapiens) facial attractiveness and sexual selection: the role of symmetry and averageness . J. Comp. Psychol. 108, 233–242.doi:

Scheib J. E., Gangestad S. W.& Thornhill R.

. 1999 Facial attractiveness, symmetry, and cues to good genes . Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 266, 1913–1917.doi:

Penton-Voak I. S., Jones B. C., Little A. C., Baker S., Tiddeman B., Burt D. M.& Perrett D. I.

. 2001 Symmetry, sexual dimorphism in facial proportions, and male facial attractiveness . Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 268, 1617–1623.doi:

Jones B. C., Little A. C., Burt D. M.& Perrett D. I.

. 2004 When facial attractiveness is only skin deep . Perception 33, 569–576.doi:

Jones B. C., Little A. C., Penton-Voak I. S., Tiddeman B. P., Burt D. M.& Perrett D. I.

. 2001 Facial symmetry and judgements of apparent health: support for a ‘good genes’ explanation of the attractiveness–symmetry relationship . Evol. Hum. Behav. 22, 417–429.doi:

Mealey L., Bridgestock R.& Townsend G.

. 1999 Symmetry and perceived facial attractiveness . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76, 151–158.doi:

. 1996 Facial asymmetry and attractiveness judgment in developmental perspective . J. Exp. Psychol. Human 22, 662–675.doi:

Rhodes G., Proffitt F., Grady J.& Sumich A.

. 1998 Facial symmetry and the perception of beauty . Psychonom. Bull. Rev. 5, 659–669.doi:

Perrett D. I., Burt D. M., Penton-Voak I. S., Lee K. J., Rowland D. A.& Edwards R.

. 1999 Symmetry and human facial attractiveness . Evol. Hum. Behav. 20, 295–307.doi:

. 2003 Evidence against perceptual bias views for symmetry preferences in human faces . Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 270, 1759–1763.doi:

. 2006 Attraction independent of detection suggests special mechanisms for symmetry preferences in human face perception . Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 3093–3099.doi:

Little A. C., Apicella C. L.& Marlowe F. W.

. 2007 Preferences for symmetry in human faces in two cultures: data from the UK and the Hadza, an isolated group of hunter–gatherers . Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 3113–3117.doi:

. 2006 Preferences for symmetry in conspecific facial shape among Macaca mulatta . Int. J. Primatol. 27, 133–145.doi:

Rhodes G., Yoshikawa S., Palermo R., Simmons L. W., Peters M., Lee K., Halberstadt J.& Crawford J. R.

. 2007 Perceived health contributes to the attractiveness of facial symmetry, averageness, and sexual dimorphism . Perception 36, 1244–1252.doi:

Thornhill R.& Gangestad S. W.

. 1993 Human facial beauty: averageness, symmetry, and parasite resistance . Hum. Nat. 4, 237–269.doi:

. 1984 Associations among proteins heterozygosity, growth rate, and developmental homeostasis . Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 15, 479–499.doi:

Roberts S. C., Little A. C., Gosling L. M., Perrett D. I., Carter V., Jones B. C., Pentonvoak I.& Petrie M.

. 2005 MHC-heterozygosity and human facial attractiveness . Evol. Hum. Behav. 26, 213–226.doi:

Lie H. C., Rhodes G.& Simmons L. W.

. 2008 Genetic diversity revealed in human faces . Evolution 62, 2473–2486.doi:

Rhodes G., Zebrowitz L. A., Clark A., Kalick S. M., Hightower A.& McKay R.

. 2001 Do facial averageness and symmetry signal health? Evol. Hum. Behav. 22, 31–46.doi:

. 1878 Composite portraits . Nature 18, 97–100.doi:

. 1990 Attractive faces are only average . Psychol. Sci. 1, 115–121.doi:

Langlois J. H., Roggman L. A.& Musselman L.

. 1994 What is average and what is not average about attractive faces . Psychol. Sci. 5, 214–220.doi:

. 2002 The role of masculinity and distinctiveness in judgments of human male facial attractiveness . Br. J. Psychol. 93, 451–464.doi:

Light L. L., Hollander S.& Kayra-Stuart F.

. 1981 Why attractive people are harder to remember . Pers. Soc. Psychol. B 7, 269–276.doi:

. 1996 Averageness, exaggeration, and facial attractiveness . Psychol. Sci. 7, 105–110.doi:

Rhodes G., Sumich A.& Byatt G.

. 1999 Are average facial configurations attractive only because of their symmetry? Psychol. Sci. 10, 52–58.doi:

Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M.& Little A. C.

. 2007 The role of symmetry in attraction to average faces . Percept. Psychophys. 69, 1273–1277.doi:

Apicella C. L., Little A. C.& Marlowe F. W.

. 2007 Facial averageness and attractiveness in an isolated population of hunter-gatherers . Perception 36, 1813–1820.doi:

. 1991 Averaged faces are attractive, but very attractive faces are not average . Psychol. Sci. 2, 123–125.doi:

Rhodes G., Yoshikawa S., Clark A., Lee K., McKay R.& Akamatsu S.

. 2001 Attractiveness of facial averageness and symmetry in non-Western populations: in search of biologically based standards of beauty . Perception 30, 611–625.doi:

. 1982 Handbook of facial growth , 2nd edn. Philadelphia, PA : Saunders . Google Scholar

. 1975 Mate selection: a selection for a handicap . J. Theor. Biol. 53, 205–214.doi:

Hillgarth N.& Wingfield J. C.

. 1997 Testosterone and immunosuppression in vertebrates: implications for parasite mediated sexual selection . Parasites and pathogens (ed.

). New York, NY : Chapman & Hall . Google Scholar

Kanda N., Tsuchida T.& Tamaki K.

. 1996 Testosterone inhibits immunoglobulin production by human peripheral blood mononuclear cells . Clin. Exp. Immunol. 106, 410–415.doi:

Yesilova Z., Ozata M., Kocar I. H., Turan M., Pekel A., Sengul A.& Caglayan Ozdemir I.

. 2000 The effects of gonadotropin treatment on the immunological features of male patients with idiopathic hypogonadotropic hypogonadism . J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 85, 66–70.doi:

. 1992 Parasites, bright males and the immunocompetence handicap . Am. Nat. 139, 603–622.doi:

Møller A. P., Christe P.& Lux E.

. 1999 Parasitism, host immune function, and sexual selection . Q. Rev. Biol. 74, 3–20.doi:

Rhodes G., Chan J., Zebrowitz L. A.& Simmons L. W.

. 2003 Does sexual dimorphism in human faces signal health? Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 270, S93–S95.doi:

2006 Facial appearance is a cue to oestrogen levels in women . Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 135–140.doi:

. 2004 High salivary testosterone is linked to masculine male facial appearance in humans . Evol. Hum. Behav. 25, 229–241.doi:

Neave N., Laing S., Fink B.& Manning J. T.

. 2003 Second to fourth digit ratio, testosterone and perceived male dominance . Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 270, 2167–2172.doi:

. 1986 Measuring the physical in physical attractiveness: quasi-experiments on the sociobiology of female facial beauty . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 50, 925–935.doi:

. 1993 Criteria of facial attractiveness in five populations . Hum. Nat. 4, 271–296.doi:

Perrett D. I., Lee K. J., Penton-Voak I. S., Rowland D. R., Yoshikawa S., Burt D. M., Henzi S. P., Castles D. L.& Akamatsu S.

. 1998 Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness . Nature 394, 884–887.doi:

Cunningham M. R., Barbee A. P.& Pike C. L.

. 1990 What do women want? Facialmetric assessment of multiple motives in the perception of male facial physical attractiveness . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 59, 61–72.doi:

. 1989 Were the physiognomists right? Personality correlates of facial babyishness . Pers. Soc. Psychol. B 15, 266–279.doi:

. 1985 Gender and the physiognomy of dominance and attractiveness . Soc. Psychol. Q. 48, 61–70.doi:

. 1983–1984 Impressions of baby-faced adults . Soc. Cogn. 2, 315–342.doi:

. 1987 Cross-cultural agreement in perceptions of babyfaced adults . J. Cross. Cult. Psychol. 18, 165–192.doi:

. 1985 Some components and consequences of a babyface . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 48, 312–323.doi:

Rhodes G., Hickford C.& Jeffery L.

. 2000 Sex-typicality and attractiveness: are supermale and superfemale faces super-attractive . Br. J. Psychol. 91, 125–140.doi:

. 2003 Testosterone increases perceived dominance but not attractiveness in human males . Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 269, 2285–2289.doi:

Little A. C., Burt D. M., Penton-Voak I. S.& Perrett D. I.

. 2001 Self-perceived attractiveness influences human female preferences for sexual dimorphism and symmetry in male faces . Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 268, 39–44.doi:

Little A. C., Jones B. C., Penton-Voak I. S., Burt D. M.& Perrett D. I.

. 2002 Partnership status and the temporal context of relationships influence human female preferences for sexual dimorphism in male face shape . Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 269, 1095–1100.doi:

2006 Correlated preferences for facial masculinity and ideal or actual partner's masculinity . Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 1355–1360.doi:

. 2006 Viewing attractive or unattractive same-sex individuals changes self-rated attractiveness and face preferences in women . Anim. Behav. 72, 981–987.doi:

Smith F. G., Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M.& Little A. C.

. 2009 Interactions between masculinity-femininity and apparent health in face preferences . Behav. Ecol. 20, 441–445.doi:

2005 Menstrual cycle, pregnancy and oral contraceptive use alter attraction to apparent health in faces . Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 347–354.doi:

Krupp D. B., DeBruine L. M.& Jones B. C.

In press. Apparent health encourages reciprocity . Evol. Hum. Behav. (doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.10.001). Google Scholar

Fink B., Grammer K.& Matts P. J.

. 2006 Visible skin color distribution plays a role in the perception of age, attractiveness, and health in female faces . Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 433–442.doi:

. 1990 Female sticklebacks use male coloration in sticklebacks and therefore avoid parasitised males . Nature 344, 330–333.doi:

. 2006 Red dominates black: agonistic signalling among head morphs in the colour polymorphic Gouldian finch . Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 949–957.doi:

Setchell J. M.& Wickings E. J.

. 2005 Dominance, status signals and coloration in male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx) . Ethology 111, 25–50.doi:

Waitt C., Little A. C., Wolfensohn S., Honess P., Brown A. P., Buchanan-Smith H. M.& Perrett D. I.

. 2003 Evidence from rhesus macaques suggests that male coloration plays a role in female primate mate choice . Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 270, S144–S146.doi:

Changizi M. A., Zhang Q.& Shimojo S.

. 2006 Bare skin, blood and the evolution of primate colour vision . Biol. Lett. 2, 217–221.doi:

. 2001 The effect of expressing anger on cardiovascular reactivity and facial blood flow in Chinese and Caucasians . Psychophysiology 38, 190–196.doi:

Waitt C., Gerald M. S., Little A. C.& Kraiselburd E.

. 2006 Selective attention toward female secondary sexual color in male rhesus macaques . Am. J. Primatol. 68, 738–744.doi:

. 2005 Do female mandrills prefer brightly colored males? Int. J. Primatol. 26, 715–735.doi:

Cuthill I. C., Hunt S., Cleary C.& Clark C.

. 1997 Colour bands, dominance, and body mass regulation in male zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) . Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 264, 1093–1099.doi:

. 2005 Red enhances human performance in contests . Nature 435, 293.doi:

Burley N., Krantzberg G.& Radman P.

. 1982 Influence of color-banding on the conspecific preferences of zebra finches . Anim. Behav. 30, 444–455.doi:

. 2007 Social perception of red suggests special role in dominance signalling . J. Evol. Psychol. 1–4, 161–168.doi:

Elliot A. J., Maier M. A., Binser M. J., Friedman R.& Pekrun R.

. 2009 The effect of red on avoidance behavior in achievement contexts . Pers. Soc. Psychol. B 35, 365–375.doi:

. 2008 Romantic red: red enhances men's attraction to women . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95, 1150–1164.doi:

Stephen I. D., Smith M. J. L., Stirrat M. R.& Perrett D. I.

. 2009 Facial skin coloration affects perceived health of human faces . Int. J. Primatol. 30, 845–857.doi:

. 2000 Facing faces: studies on the cognitive aspects of physiognomy . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 837–852.doi:

Otta E., Abrosio F. F. E.& Hoshino R. L.

. 1996 Reading a smiling face: messages conveyed by various forms of smiling . Percept. Motor Skills 82, 1111–1121. Crossref, PubMed, Google Scholar

Keating C. F., Mazur A.& Segall M. H.

. 1981 A cross-cultural exploration of physiognomic traits of dominance and happiness . Ethol. Sociobiol. 2, 41–48. Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

. 1989 Sex differences in human mate preferences: evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures . Behav. Brain Sci. 12, 1–49.doi:

. 1992 Trait inferences: sources of validity at zero acquaintance . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 62, 645–657.doi:

. 1974 The human face. London, UK : Constable . Google Scholar

Little A. C., Burt D. M.& Perrett D. I.

. 2006 What is good is beautiful: face preference reflects desired personality . Pers. Indiv. Differ. 41, 1107–1118.doi:

Botwin M. D., Buss D. M.& Shackelford T. K.

. 1997 Personality and mate preferences: five factors in mate selection and marital satisfaction . J. Pers. 65, 107–136.doi:

. 2007 Male facial appearance signals physical strength to women . Am. J. Hum. Biol. 19, 82–87.doi:

Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M., Main J. C., Little A. C., Welling L. L. M., Feinberg D. R.& Tiddeman B. P.

. 2010 Facial cues of dominance modulate the short-term gaze-cuing effect in human observers . Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 617–624.doi:

Boothroyd L. G., Jones B. C., Burt D. M., DeBruine L. M.& Perrett D. I.

. 2008 Facial correlates of sociosexuality . Evol. Hum. Behav. 29, 211–218.doi:

. 1982 Perceived age and perceived physical attractiveness . Percept. Motor Skills 54, 1251–1258. Crossref, Google Scholar

Coetzee V., Perrett D. I.& Stephen I. D.

. 2009 Facial adiposity: a cue to health? Perception 38, 1700–1711.doi:

Little A. C., Penton-Voak I. S., Burt D. M.& Perrett D. I.

. 2003 Investigating an imprinting-like phenomenon in humans: partners and opposite-sex parents have similar hair and eye colour . Evol. Hum. Behav. 24, 43–51.doi:

. 2008 The effects of facial hair manipulation on female perceptions of attractiveness, masculinity, and dominance in male faces . Pers. Indiv. Differ. 45, 373–377.doi:

. 1996 Beauty is as beauty does? Makeup and posture effects on physical attractiveness judgments . J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 26, 31–51.doi:

. 2004 Facial resemblance increases the attractiveness of same-sex faces more than other-sex faces . Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 271, 2085–2090.doi:

. 2005 Trustworthy but not lust-worthy: context-specific effects of facial resemblance . Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 919–922.doi:

Saxton T. K., Little A. C., Rowland H. M., Gao T.& Roberts S. C.

. 2009 Trade-offs between markers of absolute and relative quality in human facial preferences . Behav. Ecol. 20, 1133–1137.doi:

Rhodes G., Simmons L. W.& Peters M.

. 2005 Attractiveness and sexual behavior: does attractiveness enhance mating success? Evol. Hum. Behav. 26, 186–201.doi:

Johnston V. S., Hagel R., Franklin M., Fink B.& Grammer K.

. 2001 Male facial attractiveness: evidence for a hormone-mediated adaptive design . Evol. Hum. Behav. 22, 251–267.doi:

Jones B. C., Little A. C., Boothroyd L., DeBruine L. M., Feinberg D. R., LawSmith M. J., Cornwell R., Moore F.& Perrett D.

. 2005 Commitment to relationships and preferences for femininity and apparent health in faces are strongest on days of the menstrual cycle when progesterone level is high . Horm. Behav. 48, 283–290.doi:

Penton-Voak I. S.& Perrett D. I.

. 2000 Female preference for male faces changes cyclically: further evidence . Evol. Hum. Behav. 21, 39–48.doi:

Penton-Voak I. S., Perrett D. I., Castles D. L., Kobayashi T., Burt D. M., Murray L. K.& Minamisawa R.

. 1999 Menstrual cycle alters face preference . Nature 399, 741–742.doi:

Gangestad S. W.& Thornhill R.

. 2008 Human oestrus . Proc. R. Soc. B 275, 991–1000.doi:

Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M., Perrett D. I., Little A. C., Feinberg D. R.& Smith M. J. L.

. 2008 Effects of menstrual cycle phase on face preferences . Arch. Sex. Behav. 37, 78–84.doi:

. 2006 Cyclic variation in women's preferences for masculine traits: potential hormonal causes . Hum. Nat.-Interdiscip. Biosoc. Perspect. 17, 114–127. PubMed, Google Scholar

Feinberg D. R., Jones B. C., Law-Smith M. J., Moore F. R., DeBruine L. M., Cornwell R. E., Hillier S. G.& Perrett D. I.

. 2006 Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice . Horm. Behav. 49, 215–222.doi:

. 2005 Mating context and menstrual phase affect women's preferences for male voice pitch . Evol. Hum. Behav. 26, 388–397.doi:

Little A. C., Jones B. C.& Burriss R. P.

. 2007 Preferences for masculinity in male bodies change across the menstrual cycle . Horm. Behav. 51, 633–639.doi:

Havlicek J., Roberts S. C.& Flegr J.

. 2005 Women's preference for dominant male odour: effects of menstrual cycle and relationship status . Biol. Lett. 1, 256–259.doi:

Gangestad S. W., Garver-Apgar C. E., Simpson J. A.& Cousins A. J.

. 2007 Changes in women's mate preferences across the ovulatory cycle . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92, 151–163.doi:

Gangestad S. W., Simpson J. A., Cousins A. J., Garver-Apgar C. E.& Christensen N. P.

. 2004 Women's preferences for male behavioral displays change across the menstrual cycle . Psychol. Sci. 15, 203–207.doi:

Little A. C., Saxton T. K., Roberts S. C., Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M., Vukovic J., Perrett D. I., Feinberg D. R.& Chenore T.

. 2010 Women's preferences for masculinity in male faces are highest during reproductive age range and lower around puberty and post-menopause . Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 912–920.doi:

Vukovic J., Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M., Little A. C., Feinberg D. R.& Welling L. L. M.

. 2009 Circum-menopausal effects on women's judgements of facial attractiveness . Biol. Lett. 5, 62–64.doi:

Peters M., Simmons L. W.& Rhodes G.

. 2009 Preferences across the menstrual cycle for masculinity and symmetry in photographs of male faces and bodies . PLoS ONE 4, e4138.doi:

Little A. C., Jones B. C.& DeBruine L. M.

. 2008 Preferences for variation in masculinity in real male faces change across the menstrual cycle: women prefer more masculine faces when they are more fertile . Pers. Indiv. Diff. 45, 478–482.doi:

Welling L. L. M., Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M., Conway C. A., Smith M. J. L., Little A. C., Feinberg D., Sharp M.& Aldujaili E.

. 2007 Raised salivary testosterone in women is associated with increased attraction to masculine faces . Horm. Behav. 52, 156–161.doi:

. 2008 Women's estradiol predicts preference for facial cues of men's testosterone . Horm. Behav. 53, 14–19.doi:

Johnston L., Arden K., Macrae C. N.& Grace R. C.

. 2003 The need for speed: the menstrual cycle and person construal . Soc. Cogn. 21, 89–100.doi:

Macrae C. N., Alnwick K. A., Milne A. B.& Schloerscheidt A. M.

. 2002 Person perception across the menstrual cycle: hormonal influences on social-cognitive functioning . Psychol. Sci. 13, 532–536.doi:

Welling L. L. M., Jones B. C.& DeBruine L. M.

. 2008 Sex drive is positively associated with women's preferences for sexual dimorphism in men's and women's faces . Pers. Indiv. Differ. 44, 161–170.doi:

Welling L. L. M., Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M., Little A. C.& Smith F. G.

. 2008 Exposure to sexually attractive men decreases women's preferences for feminine faces . J. Evol. Psychol. 6, 219–230.doi:

Welling L. L. M., Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M., Smith F. G., Feinberg D. R., Little A. C.& Aldujaili E.

. 2008 Men report stronger attraction to femininity in women's faces when their testosterone levels are high . Horm. Behav. 54, 703–708.doi:

Little A. C., Jones B. C., Burt D. M.& Perrett D. I.

. 2007 Preferences for symmetry in faces change across the menstrual cycle . Biol. Psychol. 76, 209–216.doi:

. 2007 Facial symmetry detection ability changes across the menstrual cycle . Biol. Psychol. 75, 136–145.doi:

Little A. C., Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M.& Feinberg D. R.

. 2008 Symmetry and sexual dimorphism in human faces: interrelated preferences suggest both signal quality . Behav. Ecol. 19, 902–908.doi:

DeBruine L. M., Jones B. C.& Perrett D. I.

. 2005 Women's attractiveness judgments of self-resembling faces change across the menstrual cycle . Horm. Behav. 47, 379–383.doi:

DeBruine L. M., Jones B. C., Little A. C.& Perrett D. I.

. 2008 Social perception of facial resemblance in humans . Arch. Sexual Behav. 37, 64–77.doi:

. 2006 Rates of nausea and vomiting in pregnancy and dietary characteristics across populations . Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 2675–2679.doi:

Derntl B., Kryspin-Exner I., Fernbach E., Moser E.& Habel U.

. 2008 Emotion recognition accuracy in healthy young females is associated with cycle phase . Horm. Behav. 53, 90–95.doi:

Conway C. A., Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M., Welling L. L. M., Smith M. J. L., Perrett D. I., Sharp M.& Aldujaili E.

. 2007 Salience of emotional displays of danger and contagion in faces is enhanced when progesterone levels are raised . Horm. Behav. 51, 202–206.doi:

In press. Menstrual cycle and facial preferences reconsidered . Sex Roles .doi:

Scarbrough P. S.& Johnston V. S.

. 2005 Individual differences in women's facial preferences as a function of digit ratio and mental rotation ability . Evol. Hum. Behav. 26, 509–526.doi:

Boothroyd L. G.& Perrett D. I.

. 2008 Father absence, parent–daughter relationships and partner preferences . J. Evol. Psychol. 6, 187–205.doi:

Vaughn J. E., Bradley K. I., Byrd-Craven J.& Kennison S. M.

. 2010 The effect of mortality salience on women's judgments of male faces . Evol. Psychol. 8, 477–491. Crossref, PubMed, Google Scholar

Smith F. G., Jones B. C., Welling L. L. W., Little A. C., Vukovic J., Main J. C.& DeBruine L. M.

. 2009 Waist–hip ratio predicts women's preferences for masculine male faces, but not perceptions of men's trustworthiness . Pers. Indiv. Differ. 47, 476–480.doi:

Penton-Voak I. S., Little A. C., Jones B. C., Burt D. M., Tiddeman B. P.& Perrett D. I.

. 2003 Female condition influence preferences for sexual dimorphism in faces of male humans (Homo sapiens) . J. Comp. Psychol 117, 264–271.doi:

Vukovic J., Feinberg D. R., Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M., Welling L. L. M., Little A. C.& Smith F. G.

. 2008 Self-rated attractiveness predicts individual differences in women's preferences for masculine men's voices . Pers. Indiv. Differ. 45, 451–456.doi:

Vukovic J., Jones B. C., DeBruine L., Feinberg D. R., Smith F. G., Little A. C., Welling L. L. M.& Main J.

. 2010 Women's own voice pitch predicts their preferences for masculinity in men's voices . Behav. Ecol. 21, 767–772.doi:

2005 Women's physical and psychological condition independently predict their preference for apparent health in faces . Evol. Hum. Behav. 26, 451–457.doi:

Bakker T. C. M., Künzler R.& Mazzi D.

. 1999 Condition-related mate-choice in sticklebacks . Nature 401, 234.doi:

Smith F. G., Jones B. C.& DeBruine L. M.

. 2010 Individual differences in empathizing and systemizing predict variation in face preferences . Pers. Indiv. Differ. 49, 655–658.doi:

Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M., Little A. C., Conway C. A., Welling L. L. M.& Smith F.

. 2007 Sensation seeking and men's face preferences . Evol. Hum. Behav. 28, 439–446.doi:

Welling L. L. M., DeBruine L. M., Little A. C.& Jones B. C.

. 2009 Extraversion predicts individual differences in women's face preferences . Pers. Indiv. Differ. 47, 996–998.doi:

Alvergne A., Faurie C.& Raymond M.

. 2007 Differential facial resemblance of young children to their parents: who do children look like more? Evol. Hum. Behav. 28, 135–144.doi:

Nesse R. M., Silverman A.& Bortz A.

. 1990 Sex-differences in ability to recognize family resemblance . Ethol. Sociobiol. 11, 11–21. Crossref, Google Scholar

. 2004 Parental resemblance in 1-year-olds and the Gaussian curve . Evol. Hum. Behav. 25, 133–141.doi:

Bressan P.& Dal Martello M. F.

. 2002 Talis pater, talis filius: perceived resemblance and the belief in genetic relatedness . Psychol. Sci. 13, 213–218.doi:

Kaminski G., Dridi S., Graff C.& Gentaz E.

. 2009 Human ability to detect kinship in strangers' faces: effects of the degree of relatedness . Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 3193–3200.doi:

Dal Martello M. F.& Maloney L. T.

. 2006 Where are kin recognition signals in the human face . J. Vis. 6, 1356–1366.doi:

Maloney L. T.& Dal Martello M. F.

. 2006 Kin recognition and the perceived facial similarity of children . J. Vis. 6, 1047–1056.doi:

DeBruine L. M., Smith F. G., Jones B. C., Roberts S. C., Petrie M.& Spector T. D.

. 2009 Kin recognition signals in adult faces . Vis. Res. 49, 38–43.doi:

Krupp D. B., Debruine L. A.& Barclay P.

. 2008 A cue of kinship promotes cooperation for the public good . Evol. Hum. Behav. 29, 49–55.doi:

. 2002 Facial resemblance enhances trust . Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 269, 1307–1312.doi:

. 1964 The genetical evolution of social behaviour, 1 & 2 . J. Theor. Biol. 7, 1–52.doi:

. 1994 The costs of human inbreeding and their implications for variations at the DNA level . Nat. Genet. 8, 117–121.doi:

Buckingham G., DeBruine L. M., Little A. C., Welling L. L. M., Conway C. A., Tiddeman B. P.& Jones B.

. 2006 SepVisual adaptation to masculine and feminine faces influences generalized preferences and perceptions of trustworthiness . Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 381–389.doi:

. 1968 Attitudinal effects of mere exposure . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 9, 1–27.doi:

Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M., Little A. C., Conway C. A.& Feinberg D. R.

. 2006 Integrating gaze direction and expression in preferences for attractive faces . Psychol. Sci. 17, 588–591.doi:

O'Doherty J., Winston J., Critchley H., Perrett D., Burt D. M.& Dolan R. J.

. 2003 Beauty in a smile: the role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in facial attractiveness . Neuropsychologia 41, 147–155.doi:

Conway C. A., Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M.& Little A. C.

. 2008 Evidence for adaptive design in human gaze preference . Proc. R. Soc. B 275, 63–69.doi:

Conway C. A., Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M., Little A. C., Hay J., Welling L. L. M., Perrett D.& Feinberg D.

. 2008 Integrating physical and social cues when forming face preferences: differences among low and high-anxiety individuals . Soc. Neurosci. 3, 89–95.doi:

Kampe K. K. W., Frith C. D., Dolan R. J.& Frith U.

. 2001 Reward value of attractiveness and gaze: making eye contact enhances the appeal of a pleasing face, irrespective of gender . Nature 413, 589.doi:

Main J. C., Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M.& Little A. C.

. 2009 Integrating gaze direction and sexual dimorphism of face shape when perceiving the dominance of others . Perception 38, 1275–1283.doi:

. 2005 The evolution of human physical attractiveness . Ann. Rev. Anthropol. 34, 523–548.doi:

Gangestad S. W.& Simpson J. A.

. 2000 The evolution of human mating: trade-offs and strategic pluralism . Behav. Brain Sci. 23, 573–644.doi:

. 2002 Evolutionary psychology of facial attractiveness . Curr. Dir. Psychol. 11, 154–158.doi:

Penton-Voak I. S., Little A. C., Jones B. C., Burt D. M., Tiddeman B. P.& Perrett D. I.

. 2003 SepFemale condition influences preferences for sexual dimorphism in faces of male humans (Homo sapiens) . J. Comp. Psychol. 117, 264–271.doi:

. 1993 Pathogen prevalence and human mate preferences . Ethol. Sociobiol. 14, 89–96. Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

Gangestad S. W., Haselton M. G.& Buss D. M.

. 2006 Toward an integrative understanding of evoked and transmitted culture: the importance of specialized psychological design . Psychol. Inquiry 17, 138–151.doi:

DeBruine L. M., Jones B. C., Crawford J. R., Welling L. L. M.& Little A. C.

. 2010 The health of a nation predicts their mate preferences: cross-cultural variation in women's preferences for masculinized male faces . Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 2405–2410.doi:

. 2005 Sociosexuality from Argentina to Zimbabwe: a 48-nation study of sex, culture, and strategies of human mating . Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 247–311. Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar

Brooks R., Scott I., Makalov A., Kasumovic M., Clark A.& Penton-Voak I.

. 2011 National income inequality predicts women's preferences for masculinized faces better than health does . Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 810–812.doi:

DeBruine L. M., Jones B. C., Little A. C., Crawford J. R.& Welling L. L. M.

. 2011 Further evidence for regional variation in women's masculinity preferences . Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 813–814.doi:

Geary D. C., Vigil J.& Byrd-Craven J.

. 2004 Evolution of human mate choice . J. Sex Res. 41, 27–42.doi:

. 2000 Evolutionary ecology of human life history . Anim. Behav. 59, 1–10.doi:

Little A. C., Cohen D. L., Jones B. C.& Belsky J.

. 2007 Human preferences for facial masculinity change with relationship type and environmental harshness . Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 61, 967–973.doi:

. 2001 Mere exposure: a gateway to the subliminal . Curr. Dir. Psychol. 10, 224–228.doi:

. 1969 Exposure and affect: a field experiment . Psychonom. Sci. 17, 216&. Crossref, Google Scholar

Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M., Little A. C.& Feinberg D. R.

. 2007 The valence of experience with faces influences generalized preferences . J. Evol. Psychol. 5, 119–129.doi:

Kendrick K. M., Hinton M. R.& Atkins K.

. 1998 Mothers determine male sexual preferences . Nature 395, 229–230.doi:

. 1943 The innate forms of potential experience . Zietschrift Tierpsychol. 5, 234–409. Google Scholar

Bereczkei T., Gyuris P., Koves P.& Bernath L.

. 2002 Homogamy, genetic similarity, and imprinting parental influence on mate choice preferences . Pers. Indiv. Differ. 33, 677–690.doi:

Bereczkei T., Gyuris P.& Weisfeld G. E.

. 2004 Sexual imprinting in human mate choice . Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 271, 1129–1134.doi:

Perrett D. I., Penton-Voak I. S., Little A. C., Tiddeman B. P., Burt D. M., Schmidt N., Oxley R., Kinloch N.& Barrett L.

. 2002 Facial attractiveness judgements reflect learning of parental age characteristics . Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 269, 873–880.doi:

Wiszewska A., Pawlowski B.& Boothroyd L. G.

. 2007 Father–daughter relationship as a moderator of sexual imprinting: a facialmetric study . Evol. Hum. Behav. 28, 248–252.doi:

. 1980 Human assortative mating and genetic equilibrium: an evolutionary perspective . Ethol. Sociobiol. 1, 111–140. Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar

Hill C. T., Rubin Z.& Peplau L. A.

. 1976 Breakups before marriage: the end of 103 affairs . J. Soc. Issues 32, 147–168.doi:

Little A. C., Burt D. M.& Perrett D. I.

. 2006 Assortative mating for perceived facial personality traits . Pers. Indiv. Differ. 40, 973–984.doi:

. 1989 Facial resemblance in engaged and married couples . J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 6, 223–229.doi:

. 1983 Optimal outbreeding . Mate choice (ed.

), pp. 257–277. Cambridge, UK : Cambridge University Press . Google Scholar

. 1994 Symmetry, beauty and evolution . Nature 372, 169–172.doi:

. 2005 Physiologically inspired neural model for the encoding of face spaces . Neurocomputing 65, 93–101.doi:

. 1994 Female preference for symmetrical males as a by-product of selection for mate recognition . Nature 372, 172–175.doi:

Loffler G., Yourganov G., Wilkinson F.& Wilson H. R.

. 2005 fMRI evidence for the neural representation of faces . Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1386–1390.doi:

. 1991 A unified account of the effects of distinctiveness, inversion, and race in face recognition . Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 43, 161–204. Crossref, Google Scholar

. 1968 On genesis of abstract ideas . J. Exp. Psychol. 77, 353–363.doi:

Rhodes G., Halberstadt J.& Brajkovich G.

. 2001 Generalization of mere exposure effects in social stimuli . Soc. Cogn. 19, 57–70.doi:

Rhodes G., Jeffery L., Watson T. L., Jaquet E., Winkler C.& Clifford C. W. G.

. 2004 Orientation-contingent face aftereffects and implications for face-coding mechanisms . Curr. Biol. 14, 2119–2123.doi:

Leopold D. A., O'Toole A. J., Vetter T.& Blanz V.

. 2001 Prototype-referenced shape encoding revealed by high-level aftereffects . Nat. Neurosci. 4, 89–94.doi:

Webster M. A., Kaping D., Mizokami Y.& Duhamel P.

. 2004 Adaptation to natural facial categories . Nature 428, 557–561.doi:

Leopold D. A., Rhodes G., Muller K. M.& Jeffery L.

. 2005 The dynamics of visual adaptation to faces . Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 897–904.doi:

. 1999 Figural after-effects in the perception of faces . Psychonom. Bull. Rev. 6, 647–653.doi:

Rhodes G., Jeffery L., Watson T. L., Clifford C. W. G.& Nakayama K.

. 2003 Fitting the mind to the world: face adaptation and attractiveness aftereffects . Psychol. Sci. 14, 558–566.doi:

DeBruine L. M., Welling L. L. M., Jones B. C.& Little A. C.

. 2010 Opposite effects of visual versus imagined presentation of faces on subsequent sex perception . Vis. Cogn. 18, 816–828.doi:

Jones B. C., Feinberg D. R., Bestelmeyer P. E. G., Debruine L. M.& Little A. C.

. 2010 Adaptation to different mouth shapes influences visual perception of ambiguous lip speech . Psychon. Bull. Rev. 17, 522–528.doi:

Little A. C., DeBruine L. M.& Jones B. C.

. 2005 Sex-contingent face after-effects suggest distinct neural populations code male and female faces . Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 2283–2287.doi:

Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M.& Little A. C.

. 2008 Adaptation reinforces preferences for correlates of attractive facial cues . Vis. Cogn. 16, 849–858.doi:

Little A. C., Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M.& Caldwell C. A.

. 2011 Social learning and human mate preferences: a potential mechanism for generating and maintaining between-population diversity in attraction . Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 366, 366–375.doi:

. 2005 Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution . Chicago, IL : University of Chicago Press . Google Scholar

. 2005 Sexual selection: copycat mating in birds . Curr. Biol. 15, R626–R628.doi:

. 2004 Influences of social learning on mate-choice decisions . Learn. Behav. 32, 105–113.doi:

. 2000 The imitation factor: evolution beyond the gene . New York, NY : Free Press . Google Scholar

. 2005 Social learning in animals: empirical studies and theoretical models . Bioscience 55, 489–499.doi:

. 1992 Reversal of female mate choice by copying in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata) . Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 249, 179–184.doi:

. 1993 Female mate copying in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata): age-dependent effects . Behav. Ecol. 4, 289–292.doi:

Godin J. G. J., Herdman E. J. E.& Dugatkin L. A.

. 2005 Social influences on female mate choice in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata: generalized and repeatable trait-copying behaviour . Anim. Behav. 69, 999–1005.doi:

. 2002 Mate choice copying in the sailfin molly, Poecilia latipinna, in the wild . Anim. Behav. 63, 943–949.doi:

. 1998 Mate-choice copying in Japanese quail, Coturnix coturnix japonica . Anim. Behav. 55, 545–552.doi:

Swaddle J. P., Cathey M. G., Correll M.& Hodkinson B. P.

. 2005 Socially transmitted mate preferences in a monogamous bird: a non-genetic mechanism of sexual selection . Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 1053–1058.doi:

. 2000 ‘Culture' in quail: social influences on mate choices of female Coturnix japonica . Anim. Behav. 59, 975–979.doi:

. 2003 Human mate choice and the wedding ring effect: are married men more attractive? Hum. Nat.-Interdiscip. Biosoc. Perspect. 14, 267–276. PubMed, Google Scholar

. 2006 Are all the taken men good? An indirect examination of mate-choice copying in humans . Can. Med. Assoc. J. 175, 1573–1574.doi:

. 2008 The mere presence of opposite-sex others on judgments of sexual and romantic desirability: opposite effects for men and women . Pers. Soc. Psychol. B 34, 635–647.doi:

Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M., Little A. C., Burriss R. P.& Feinberg D. R.

. 2007 Social transmission of face preferences among humans . Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 899–903.doi:

. 1973 Radiating beauty: effects of having a physically attractive partner on person perception . J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 28, 218–224.doi:

Little A. C., Burriss R. P., Jones B. C., DeBruine L. M.& Caldwell C. A.

. 2008 Social influence in human face preference: men and women are influenced more for long-term than short-term attractiveness decisions . Evol. Hum. Behav. 29, 140–146.doi:


Special offers and product promotions

Review

?[W]ill have a wide audience that ranges from general readers and lower-division undergraduates to faculty and professionals.?-Choice

"ÝW¨ill have a wide audience that ranges from general readers and lower-division undergraduates to faculty and professionals."-Choice

"[W]ill have a wide audience that ranges from general readers and lower-division undergraduates to faculty and professionals."-Choice

About the Author

GILLIAN RHODES is Professor of Psychology at the University of Western Australia and the author of Superportraits: Caricatures and Recognition. She is a recipient of the New Zealand Psychological Society's Hunter Award for Excellence in Research and is on the editorial boards of Psychonomic Bulletin, The British Journal of Psychology, and the New Zealand Journal of Psychology.


Attraction is the power or ability to evoke interest, liking, or pleasure for something or someone. In general, it can also be considered as a force which pulls or draws one object to another. Psychology of attraction is said to follow the Law of Attraction. This law is explained briefly by the fact like-attracts-like. The law is further explained as, things of similar kind tend to attract to each other. The same law holds good for thoughts too. Positive thoughts lead us to positive experiences in life and similarly negative thoughts are responsible for negative experiences in life. To make the law of attraction work, we need to focus, believe and have full faith on what we desire. Attraction can be of many kinds, such as interpersonal attraction, physical attraction, sexual attraction, tourist attraction, and amusement park attraction.

Interpersonal attraction is a force that acts between two people which tends to draw them closer, which further leads to romantic relationships or friendship, and resists the separation between them. Interpersonal attraction is measured by the ‘Interpersonal Attraction Judgment Scale’. In this measurement procedure, qualities of the one who is attracted as well as the qualities of the attractor as well as the situation and personality is taken into account.

Physical attraction is the attractiveness caused by a person’s pleasing physical appearance. Researchers found that there is a direct relationship between general intelligence and physical appearance. Men in general, are attracted to women with a height shorter than they are and youthful appearance, along with full breasts and lips, symmetrical face, and a low ratio of waste-hip. Women, are attracted to men, taller than them, masculine dimorphism, broad shoulders and a torso having V-like shape.

Sexual attraction refers to a person’s ability to attract another by arousing his/her sexual desire. The notion of sexual attractiveness is a relative aspect as the measure of getting attracted to someone sexually can vary from person to person. It depends on the person’s interest, sexual orientation and perception. We can be attracted to a person’s looks, clothing, smell, voice, hair, style, practically anything related with the physical appearance.

Tourist attraction, on the other hand is a place of interest which is frequently visited by the tourist for its cultural value, natural beauty or historical significance.

Amusement park attraction is a location, which is artificially built in order to entertain and give enjoyment to a large group of people through its attractive events, rides and many more.

In a scientific perspective, it is the genes which decide things that would seem attractive to us. But you might find certain things which are liked or loved by almost everyone. Some types of shapes, colors, organization and symmetry seem attractive to many. In web designing, shapes like triangle (represents balance, energy, strength), circle (represents family, warmth, love) and squares (logic, security, order) play a major role. Colors like red (arouses emotions like anger, fear, passion) and blue (trustworthiness and loyalty) are used in marketing quiet often. It has been found that clashing colors repulse viewers. Symmetrical features are found to be more attractive than asymmetric ones to people, especially babies.


Facial Symmetry and Attractiveness

One of the leading aspects used to measure conventional attractiveness scientifically is facial symmetry. Typically, this is measured by manipulating an original photo of a person (we are all at least a little asymmetric, no person is perfectly symmetrical) into a perfectly symmetric version of their face. This manipulated, symmetric image is then presented to test subject along with the original photo. Subjects are then asked to indicate which face is more attractive, usually indicating the symmetrical version. (These findings have been replicated in multiple studies.) Though these results indicate that people prefer and perceive the more symmetric faces as attractive, there has been considerable debate about why this is.

There have been two theories of substance proposed by researchers to explain the preference for symmetrical faces:

The Evolutionary Advantage theory proposed that symmetrical faces are perceived as more attractive because the symmetry indicates good health in an individual. Everyone’s genes are designed to develop a face perfectly symmetrical, but as we grow, develop, and then age, disease, infections, and parasites cause imperfection in our appearance (asymmetry). Thus, those that have less asymmetry and imperfections, are perceived as having better and stronger immune systems to withstand the infections and parasites that occur naturally. So, symmetry is a good indicator of a person having good genes to pass on their offspring. Under the Evolutionary Advantage view of symmetric preferences, we have evolved to prefer symmetry and perceive it as attractive because over human history we have consistently and constantly preferred healthier individuals for mates. In sum, the Evolutionary Advantage view suggests that attraction to symmetric individuals reflects an attraction to healthy individuals who would be good mates.

The second theory to explain the preference for facial symmetry is Perceptual Bias. This theory suggests that the human visual system may be “hard wired” in a way that makes it much easier to process symmetrical stimuli than asymmetrical stimuli. If this is true, the ease of processing symmetrical stimuli would cause us to naturally prefer them to asymmetrical stimuli. Under this view, preferences for symmetrical faces would be no different than for any other object. So according to this, as well as preferring symmetrical faces, humans would also prefer more symmetrical objects of any kind. This has been supported as it has been found that people much prefer symmetrical pieces of abstract art and sculptures to asymmetrical ones.

Little and Jones (2003) did a study to investigate why people prefer symmetric faces to asymmetric ones, by testing and attempting to apply predictions from both the Evolutionary Advantage theory and Perceptual Bias. Previous studies found that the symmetric preference is stronger for attractiveness of opposite sex than same sex. Little and Jones found that the manipulated, symmetric faces were judged more attractive when shown the right way up, but not when the faces were inverted. These findings suggest that symmetry is more important in mate choice stimuli than in other stimuli, supporting the Evolutionary Advantage theory and presenting multiple difficulties for the Perception Bias theory (if symmetry of any kind was preferred then the more symmetrical face would have been indicated as more attractive both the right way up AND when inverted).

If anyone is interested in learning more, you can benefit from taking a class or just researching Penn State’s very own Dr. Mark Shriver, a geneticist, who conducts research in Brazil on facial symmetry. Though ongoing, Shriver’s research has measured thousands of Brazilian (and other ethnicities) faces in facial symmetry, judging their scientific attractiveness and therefore contributing the most evidence towards the idea that mixed race people are more attractive — in this case attraction is not subjective, it is purely measure with symmetry. Shriver teaches many higher level ANTH classes, but if anyone is interested, I suggest starting with ANTH 021 – Biological Anthropology.


Yes, You Really Do Have a 'Type,' Science Says

I s beauty really in the eye of the beholder? A new study says yes, discovering that 50% of people’s preferences for faces is unique to them, and who we find attractive is most strongly influenced by our life experiences.

“If you were to rate faces [for attractiveness] and I were to rate the same faces, we would agree about 50% of the time,” says study author Jeremy Wilmer, an assistant professor of psychology at Wellesley College whose new research was published Thursday in the journal Current Biology.

The two-part study first had around 35,000 people rate faces for attractiveness on the website Testmybrain.org. Prior research has suggested that people generally agree that symmetrical faces are better-looking. That, and other factors, may account for the 50% consensus on beauty.

As for the other 50% of faces? That’s where the science really gets interesting.

In the second part of the study, the researchers studied the facial preferences of 547 pairs of identical twins and 214 pairs of fraternal twins. The hope was that by studying people who are genetically the same (or at least very similar) and who grew up and in the same environment, they would gain some insight into the nature or nurture effects of where the “eye of the beholder” variation in attractiveness perception comes from.

If something is really influenced by genes you would expect identical twins to be more similar to each other than the fraternal twins, Wilmer says. On the other hand, if family environment is highly influential, the researchers would expect fraternal twins to be quite similar to each other in preferences. But that’s not what they found.

“In our case, we found that even though identical twins share all of their genes and their family environment they were really, really different from each other in their facial aesthetic preferences,” says Wilmer.

It’s likely personal experiences inform who we find attractive, and because they are so personal, we can greatly differ from someone who&mdashon paper&mdashis quite similar to us, the research suggests.

So what types of experiences influence who we find attractive? The researchers didn’t study this in their paper, but study author Laura Germine of Massachusetts General Hospital says that based on previous literature on this subject, they have some theories.

“[Prior research] has found things like, if you take a face and you pair it with positive information, that face then looks more attractive, and faces that are similar to it also look more attractive and vice versa,” she says. “So you can imagine as you go through life and you form relationships and have friends and people you have a more positive relationship with, you may come to find their face characteristics more attractive, and then other people who look similar to them are then more attractive to you.”

“Exposure to certain faces makes them seem more attractive,” she adds. That means a face that is very different from a face that you have never seen before tends to be judged as less attractive. It also means the kinds of faces you are exposed to in your work environment, in your relationships or even the face of your spouse could shift the kind of faces that you find attractive, she says.

Basically, who you find attractive may be less about where you grew up and where you went to school and more influenced by experiences that are very unique to you. More research is still needed, but the study suggests beauty really may be in the eye of the beholder.


Evaluation: Issues & Debates

Physical attractiveness seems to be an important factor in forming relationships across cultures. For example, Cunningham et al. (1995) found that white, Asian and Hispanic males, despite being from different cultures, rated females with prominent cheekbones, small noses and large eyes as highly attractive. This universality of findings suggests that using attractiveness as a decisive factor in choosing a partner might be a genetically reproduced mechanism, aiding sexual selection. This gives support to the nature side of nature-nurture debate as it shows that human behaviour is mainly a result of biological rather than environmental influences.

On the other hand, the matching hypothesis may be suffering from a beta-bias, as it assumes that men and women are very similar in their view of the importance of physical attractiveness. Research, however, suggests that this may not be the case. For example, Meltzer et al. (2014) found that men rate their long-term relationships more satisfying if their partner is physically attractive, while for women their partner’s attractiveness didn’t have a significant impact on relationship satisfaction. This shows that there are significant gender differences in how important appearance is for attraction.

The matching hypothesis is a theory that is based on a nomothetic approach to studying human behaviour. It tries to generate behavioural laws applicable to all people however, as studies above suggest, there are significant individual differences in the importance of physical attractiveness to one’s choice of a partner. Therefore, explanations based on the idiographic approach (studying individual cases in detail, without trying to generate universal rules) may be more appropriate for studying romantic relationships.


"Why are black women less attractive?" asks Psychology Today

By Natasha Lennard
Published May 17, 2011 11:30PM (EDT)

Supermodel, television personality and famed beauty, Tyra Banks, "objectively" less attractive?

Shares

An article posted online on Monday by Psychology Today provoked controversy and cries of racism. The title of evolutionary psychologist Dr. Satoshi Kanazawa's piece: "Why Are Black Women Less Physically Attractive Than Other Women?"

His argument is that there is a set of data, which shows black women to be "objectively" less attractive than white, Asian or Native American women, but that the same data does not find black men less attractive than men of other races. Kanazawa accepts this data and then tries to explain why it is the case. He suggests that black people have more testosterone than other races, and so possess "more masculine features." He states too that women are "objectively" more attractive than men, so if black women have more masculine features, this explains why they are rated less attractive.

Following outraged responses, Psychology Today changed the article's title to "Why Are Black Women Rated Less Physically Attractive Than Other Women?" and then took it down altogether, although you can still read the "study" in its entirety here.

Now, in the tagline to his Psychology Today blog -- the Scientific Fundamentalist -- Kanazawa warns, "If what I say is wrong (because it is illogical or lacks credible scientific evidence), then it is my problem. If what I say offends you, it is your problem."

Well, by Kanazawa's standards then, his recent article is most certainly his problem (as well as a problem for Psychology Today and the academic institutions he is affiliated with -- the London School of Economics and London's Birckbeck College).

First, let's look at the data Kanazawa uses, which allegedly shows "objectively" that black women are less attractive than non-black women. It turns out the evidence is just the opinions of a group of researchers who worked for a large study called Add Health. The researchers were asked to rate the study’s subjects (men and women from different races) according to how attractive they found them (1=very unattractive, 2= unattractive, 3=about average, 4=attractive, 5=very attractive). As Colorlines notes:

 I'm confused about how these data are objective. Did some bias-free robots from the utopian ether descend upon each testing site to perform this portion of the evaluation? Or were the interviewers human beings, subject to the same racism, sexism, ablelism, homophobia, transphobia, xenophobia, fat phobia and whateverthehellelsephobia that undergirds beauty standards?

So, Kanazawa essentially seems to be asking, "Why are black women less attractive according to standards of attractiveness which find black women less attractive" – he begs his own question.

Kanazawa's conclusion -- that it is testosterone that makes black women less physically attractive -- adds another layer of bad logic. "Recall," he states (without qualification), "that women on average are more physically attractive than men" -- ergo black women, who have more testosterone and so look more manly, are less attractive.

Whole university departments are devoted to the complexities of how gender is considered and how notions of attractiveness problematically sit around gender divisions. Kanazawa, however, in his use of the terms "attractive," "women" and "men," ignores the fact that these issues are the subjects of much ongoing debate and analysis.

And the evolutionary psychologist does not point to the problems involved with generalizing about races -- which, of course, categorize millions of very different people.

Groups like "LSE: Home of the Racist Academic. Say No" have formed in opposition to Kanazawa (who has a history of racially biased research). They are demanding an apology from Psychology Today and have called for Kanazawa's employer universities to reconsider his positions, the U.K. site Voice Online reported.

Natasha Lennard

Natasha Lennard is an assistant news editor at Salon, covering non-electoral politics, general news and rabble-rousing. Follow her on Twitter @natashalennard, email [email protected]

MORE FROM Natasha LennardFOLLOW natashalennardLIKE Natasha Lennard


Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness

Testosterone-dependent secondary sexual characteristics in males may signal immunological competence 1 and are sexually selected for in several species 2 , 3 . In humans, oestrogen-dependent characteristics of the female body correlate with health and reproductive fitness and are found attractive 4,5,6 . Enhancing the sexual dimorphism of human faces should raise attractiveness by enhancing sex-hormone-related cues to youth and fertility in females 5 , 7,8,9,10,11 , and to dominance and immunocompetence in males 5 , 12 , 13 . Here we report the results of asking subjects to choose the most attractive faces from continua that enhanced or diminished differences between the average shape of female and male faces. As predicted, subjects preferred feminized to average shapes of a female face. This preference applied across UK and Japanese populations but was stronger for within-population judgements, which indicates that attractiveness cues are learned. Subjects preferred feminized to average or masculinized shapes of a male face. Enhancing masculine facial characteristics increased both perceived dominance and negative attributions (for example, coldness or dishonesty) relevant to relationships and paternal investment. These results indicate a selection pressure that limits sexual dimorphism and encourages neoteny in humans.


References

Bhogal, M. S., Galbraith, N., & Manktelow, K. (2016). Physical attractiveness and altruism in two modified dictator games. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 38(4), 212–222.

Bhogal, M. S., Galbraith, N., & Manktelow, K. (2017). Physical attractiveness, altruism and cooperation in an ultimatum game. Current Psychology, 36(3), 549–555.

Bhogal, M. S. (2019). Altruism Advertises Cooperativeness. In: Shackelford T., Weekes-Shackelford V. (eds) Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science. Springer, Cham.

Ferraro, R. F. (2019). Special Issue on Psychology of Prosocial Behavior. Current Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00336-4.

Ehlebracht, D., Stavrova, O., Fetchenhauer, D., & Farrelly, D. (2018). The synergistic effect of prosociality and physical attractiveness on mate desirability. British Journal of Psychology, 109(3), 517–537.

Farrelly, D. (2019). Indirect Benefits of Altruism. In: Shackelford T., Weekes-Shackelford V. (eds) Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science. Springer, Cham.

Margana, L., Bhogal, M. S., Bartlett, J. E., & Farrelly, D. (2019). The roles of heroism, altruism, and physical attractiveness in female mate choice. Personality and Individual Differences, 137, 126–130.


Watch the video: Τα 5 βιβλία της ζωής μου! (January 2022).